Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Social Loafing: Causes, Effects, and Possible Solutions, Lecture notes of Psychology

The phenomenon of social loafing, a behavior where individuals decrease their effort when working in a group. The research covers the origins of social loafing, its impact on physical and cognitive tasks, causes and deterrents, partner adaptation, and social loafing in modern technology. The document also discusses the implications of social loafing for individuals, social institutions, and societies, and suggests possible solutions for group leaders.

Typology: Lecture notes

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/12/2022

alfred67
alfred67 🇺🇸

4.9

(20)

75 documents

1 / 10

Toggle sidebar

Related documents


Partial preview of the text

Download Social Loafing: Causes, Effects, and Possible Solutions and more Lecture notes Psychology in PDF only on Docsity! Social Loafing: A Review of the Literature Ashley Simms Texas Wesleyan University Tommy Nichols Texas Wesleyan University Social loafing is a phenomenon that has been discussed and researched since 1913. Though it has been long examined, recent technological developments offer ample opportunity for further study. This paper summarizes its long history of research and offers several propositions for future research. INTRODUCTION In 1913 a phenomenon was found that, at the time, did not receive sufficient attention. Maximilien Ringelmann, a French agricultural engineer, observed that when a group of people collectively pulled on a rope, the output was less than when group members individually pulled on the rope (Kravitz and Martin, 1986; Ringelmann, 1913). The results of this finding were not considered further until 1974 when Ingham, Levinger, Graves, and Peckham recreated the experiment. The term “social loafing” was coined for the discovery that participants working in groups exert less effort than participants working individually. It was described as having a detrimental effect on individuals and the institutions associated with them (Latane, Williams, & Harkins, 1979). From there, the research evolved into five distinct categories: 1) establishing the existence of social loafing in both physical and cognitive group projects, 2) causes and deterrents of social loafing, 3) partner adaptation to group member social loafing (such as the “Sucker Effect”), 4) social loafing as a positive mechanism, and finally 5) social loafing in modern technology. ESTABLISHING SOCIAL LOAFING EXISTS The origins of social loafing begin with “The Ringelmann Effect,” which describes the tendency for individuals to lower their productivity when in a group (Ringlemann, 1913); Ingham, Levinger, Graves and Peckham relabeled this effect “social loafing” when they were successful in demonstrating individual effort declines in a curvilinear fashion when people work as a group or only believe they are working in a group (Ingham, Levinger, Graves, & Peckham, 1974). Social loafing literature blossomed as these findings gave way to more questions: is the problem coordination and not motivation? Since the experiment involved groups created for the experiment, would established groups have the same results? What exactly causes social loafing? Is it because group dynamics allow an individual the ability to “hide in the crowd”? Does social loafing only exist in physical activities? Or does it also happen when groups are performing cognitive tasks? These questions paved the way for much more research over the years. 58 Journal of Management Policy and Practice vol. 15(1) 2014 Social loafing had already been established in physical tasks, but would the same results be found in cognitive group tasks? In 1977, Petty, Harkings, Williams, and Latane conducted a study to demonstrate cognitive tasks were just as susceptible to social loafing as physical tasks. The experiment was divided into three groups. The first group of students was asked to edit a poem, believing they were solely responsible for the task. The second group consisted of a group of four, and the third group believed they were in a group of sixteen. The results support that social loafing does occur in cognitive tasks (Petty, Harkings, Williams, & Latane, 1977). The study was limited, however, in that no rewards were offered to the students for doing a good job and no individual feedback was given, which gave way to the question: would having rewards and feedback help prevent social loafing from occurring? Five years after the landmark Ingham et al. (1974) study, Latane, Williams, and Harkins replicated the experiment with cheering and clapping. This research contributed two important advances: they demonstrated Ringelmann’s results were replicable, and because of the simplistic nature of yelling and clapping, they also made clear that the results of decreased efforts were not simply because of coordination problems between group members or difficulty of the task. Support of the effect only led to more questions: when and why does social loafing take place? Because groups are such a huge part of everyone’s life, what can be done to prevent it from happening? Realizing the far-reaching implications of these findings, they famously came to the conclusion that social loafing is “a kind of social disease… it has negative consequences for individuals, social institutions, and societies” (Latane et al., 1979, p.831). Although the researchers described social loafing in such harsh terms, they did mention in their discussion that people may have decided to loaf in groups because they wanted to save their energy for times when they would need to work as an individual, and would be able to earn rewards (Harkins, Latane, & Williams, 1980). This adaptive look at social loafing was not fully explored until thirty years later. CIRCUMSTANCES THAT ENCOURAGE SOCIAL LOAFING AND THOSE THAT DETER IT Once social loafing studies were replicated, researchers began looking into antecedents and deterrents. Williams, Harkins, and Latane, expanded their cheering experiment with an added variable: if people thought their individual effort was able to be measured, would they have less of a tendency to loaf (Williams, Harkins, & Latane, 1981)? In order to convince participants that their individual efforts would be measured, microphones were attached to each person. The results suggest that the belief their personal efforts are measured does discourage social loafing. In the second experiment they performed, they demonstrated that individual output measurability is important even when someone is working alone. Participants were told their output could not be measured, and even when working alone, the results revealed that participants’ productivity decreased; they loafed. So what can be done to discourage social loafing when individual output cannot be measured? All previous research focused on the measurement of output. Harkins and Petty (1982) set out to discover if making the task more interesting or harder would decrease social loafing. Their data suggest that when people are given a difficult task, they work on it just as hard in a group as they would individually. Additionally, when an individual is given a task that they have much knowledge about, or that they are skilled at, social loafing is reduced (Harkins & Petty, Effects of Task Difficulty and Task Uniqueness on Social Loafing, 1982). They also took a look at another possibility that could reduce social loafing: an individual seeing their contribution as unique. All previous research studies combined the group task, but Harkins and Petty changed that variable and told group members their personal contribution was unique to them. Their experiment demonstrated that individuals are less likely to loaf when they feel the contribution they make is unique, and no other group member can contribute the skills to the task that they can (Harkins & Petty, Effects of Task Difficulty and Task Uniqueness on Social Loafing, 1982). Robbins (1995), however, found that social loafing did occur despite the use of thought-provoking tasks that provided the chance for unique contributions. Zaccaro (1984) studied the role of task attractiveness in social loafing in an attempt to expand and refine the work by Latane et al. (1979). They found that group interaction, task commitment, and identifiability are possible deterrents of social loafing. They suggest that group cohesiveness (e.g. high Journal of Management Policy and Practice vol. 15(1) 2014 59 individual differences regarding social loafing (e.g. Barua, Sophi-Lee & Whinston, 1995; George, 1992; and George, 1995). They hypothesized that an individual preference for group work is negatively related to social loafing. They also hypothesized that when an individual’s winning orientation, “a concern for favorable social comparisons and positive relative positions” (Stark, Shaw, & Duffy, 2007, p. 717) is low the negative relationship between preferences for group work will be stronger. A person with low winning orientation is not concerned with negative social comparisons, and thus would have no motivation to avoid loafing given the opportunity. They additionally hypothesized that when task interdependence is high, even an individual with low winning orientation and a negative outlook on group work is less likely to loaf. As expected, preference for group work is negatively related to loafing, both from self-evaluations and peer-evaluations, as individuals are more willing to highlight their own weaknesses and less willing to highlight the weaknesses of others. The hypothesis about winning orientation and social loafing was only supported in the self-evaluations, not the peer-evaluations. They also found that the negative effects of low preference for group work could be contradicted with a high winning orientation. On the other hand, the task interdependence hypothesis was supported with the peer-evaluations, but not with the self- evaluations. Their findings confirm that social loafing is not only situational, but is also complexly related to a person’s psychology, expanding on Liden et al., 2004. As with previous studies, only students were used, limiting the results of the study. There may be a big difference in results when class assignments are given grades, compared to real life work situations. They admitted that their peer-evaluations had several weaknesses, and future researchers should look at ways of improving it. The study also demonstrated that gender is related to social loafing in both self and peer-evaluations, and they recommended these aspects be looked at more carefully in future research. Doing more research on the impact psychology has on social loafing, Klehe and Anderson attempted to answer a question posed by Williams, Karkins, and Latane in 1981, and was also touched on by Charbonnier, Huguet, Brauer, and Monteil in their study in 1998: does culture have an impact on social loafing? Along with culture, they also studied personality, social, work, and organizational psychology. They used three personality dimensions: conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness. Contrary to their hypotheses, none of the personality traits have any influence on an individual’s propensity to loaf in a situation (Klehe & Anderson, 2007). The cultural dimensions of individualism versus collectivism and power distance do affect an individual’s tendency to loaf, as hypothesized. People from individualistic cultures are more inclined to loaf, and people from collectivistic cultures have more motivation when working in groups. As hypothesized, individuals with high power distance are more likely to loaf than individuals with lower power distance (Klehe & Anderson, 2007). The relationship between power distance and social loafing is so strong, they recommended more testing to determine if an individualistic culture orientation actually causes loafing, or if power distance alone is enough to predict loafing. There were two major limitations of the experiment. The first was the common problem that only undergraduate students were used. The second was that they used a “paper people” design in order to get an impression if their hypotheses were on the right track or not, and they cannot be sure they would get the same results in real life scenarios. Like Klehe and Anderson’s 2007 study, Tan and Tan in 2008 looked at the effects personality has on social loafing. They also studied the relationship between social loafing and organizational citizenship behavior, as well as motives and contextual factors. Their first hypothesis that conscientiousness is negatively related to social loafing was supported. The second hypothesis that organizational citizenship behavior is negatively related to social loafing was also supported. In addition they found that organizational citizenship behavior is positively related to conscientiousness, and felt responsibility relates negatively to social loafing. Their third hypothesis, that “the contextual factors of task visibility, task interdependence, group cohesiveness, and felt responsibility are negatively related to social loafing” (Tan & Tan, 2008, p. 96), however, was not supported. Because most research before this was done on contextual factors (e.g. task visibility Geroge, 1992; Jones, 1984; Latane et al., 1979; task interdependence Liden et al., 2004; Pearce & Gregersen, 1991; Williams & Karau, 1991; and group cohesiveness Karau & Hart, 1998), this study helped show that an individual’s personality also plays a part in their social loafing, which can help employers curtail loafing in the work place when they are 62 Journal of Management Policy and Practice vol. 15(1) 2014 recruiting and hiring employees. As in most of the other studies, a major problem with this study was that all participants were undergraduate students in an introductory management class. A strength of this study over others is that it spanned over a semester (three months) and did not study students doing tasks in the lab, but of them working in groups on assignments for the management class outside a laboratory setting. SUCKER EFFECT AND OTHER PARTNER STUDIES While some researchers were looking at the contextual and personality factors that caused individuals to engage in social loafing, others were taking a look at the dynamics of social loafing and the effect that it had on partners of the loafer. In 1983, Kerr hypothesized that when one partner engaged in social loafing, their partner would in turn reduce their efforts in the project in order to not be “taken for a sucker.” His research uncovered the “sucker effect,” and even in some severe cases saw that partners would rather fail at a task than feel like they were being “taken for a sucker.” He also found, however, this is not always the case; the effect only appears in partners who have the ability to do the work, but simply do not put all their effort into the job. If someone does not have the ability to do the work, their partner does not feel the need to reduce their own effort in order to match. Additionally, they found that if a partner constantly goes above and beyond in a project, partners are more likely to start to “free ride” because they are still guaranteed the success of the project based on the over achieving partner’s efforts. They also discovered that males are more likely to engage in social loafing than females (Kerr, 1983). In 1985, Jackson and Harkins, further examined people lowering their efforts when they have a partner who is engaging in social loafing. They hypothesized that when an individual thinks their partner is going to loaf, they will lower their own efforts in order to match their partner’s, and try to maintain equity in effort. They also found, like Harkins and Petty did in 1982, task difficulty helped eliminate loafing, or by assigning specific tasks to each person (Jackson & Harkins, 1985). Like most other studies, a limitation was that all the participants were not only undergrads, but also were entirely female students in a required Introductory Psychology class. However, in 1991, Williams and Karau found the opposite effect; when an individual knows that their group members are going to loaf, they step up and shoulder the burden for the loafer, also referred to as social compensation. They found that trust factors moderate social loafing and social compensation. Further, when someone believes their partner is competent and a hard worker, they have more of a tendency to take advantage of that and engage in loafing themselves. And although Jackson and Harkins (1985) found that the reason for a partner’s poor performance determines an adjustment of output to keep everyone’s work equal, Williams and Karau (1991) found that it does not matter why someone’s partner performed poorly, and does not affect a social compensation response. Like others studies, they found that the importance of the task plays a big motivating factor in the effort put forth by participants (Williams & Karau, 1991). They urged more research to see if the compensation effect will remain constant the longer the group works together, or if it will start to decline after several projects and participants grow weary of compensating for a group member expending less effort. Participants of the study were all undergrad introductory psychology students. Harcum and Badura in 1991 investigated just how deep social loafing can occur. They hypothesized that loafing will not only occur when an individual is isolated from their partner, but also when an individual just thinks they have a partner while completing a task. Eighty-one introductory psychology course participants were placed in rooms and told they had a partner working with them in the other room to complete a “tapping task and/or a word search task” (Harcum & Badura, 1991, p. 631). Not only did this study demonstrate people loaf while just thinking they have a partner, but because of the set up of the task, it also nullified previous questions about whether social loafing is caused due to lack of coordination with fellow group members (Hardy & Latane, 1986; Latane et al., 1979; and Petty et al., 1977). Eder and Eisenberger (2008) examined the role of perceived organizational support (POS) in coworker withdrawal behavior. In a manufacturing organization, they found that POS eliminates the relation between work group and individual tardiness; when POS is low, work group and individual tardiness is correlated. In a second study, they discovered in a retail sales organization that POS reduces the relation between work Journal of Management Policy and Practice vol. 15(1) 2014 63 group withdrawal and individual withdrawal. These studies support previous research (Eisenberger, Huntington, Hutchinson, & Sowa, 1986) who found a negative relationship between absenteeism and POS. Overall, when employees perceive that their organization values their contributions and cares about their well-being, they are less likely to engage in withdrawal behaviors, even when these behaviors are displayed by their coworkers (Eder & Eisenberger, 2008). While not explicitly mentioning social loafing in the article, social loafers likely engage in withdrawal behaviors such as taking undeserved work breaks, spending time in idle conversations, and neglecting aspects of the job one is obligated to perform (all constructs of the withdrawal measure used it the study). Kidwell and Bennett (1993) suggest that constructs such as shirking, social loafing, or free riding all contain a common thread, the propensity to withhold effort and can be considered one construct. Hung, Chi, and Lu (2009) looked at social loafing as an antecedent to counterproductive work behaviors. They found that perceived coworker loafing triggers revenge motives towards both the organization and individuals, which in turn triggers still more employees to engage in counterproductive work behaviors. These researchers suggest lowering the possibility for social loafing by communicating the significance of the loafer’s jobs or goals (Liden et al., 2004). This study, however, was limited by a small sample size in Taiwan, a collectivistic culture. UNIQUE STUDIES OF NOTE Social Loafing as Positive and Adaptive In the Latane, Williams, and Harkins 1979 study, and in the Jackson and Harkins study in 1985, it was suggested that social loafing might not be entirely bad. But it was not until 2009 that this theory was actually looked at and researched in depth. The study explored social loafing as a way for employees to conserve their energy so that when working individually, they were not completely burned out. Bluhm (2009) proposed that when working in a group, the task was still accomplished to everyone’s satisfaction, but individuals did not have to work as hard as they would have if they had been working on the task alone. This enabled them to expend more resources when they are working on an individual project. This also caused an employee less stress and strain when working on a group project, giving them more energy to put forth when working alone. They also proposed that eliminating group work would actually have a negative effect on employees as well as the company because it would lead to increase stress and strain on employees. Although strictly theoretical, his paper encouraged future research on this aspect of social loafing (Bluhm, 2009). Social Loafing in Technology Technology in the workplace has changed the dynamic of working in groups. Groups can now be formed with people in different states, as well as from different countries. In 2010, Alnuaimi, Robert, and Maruping took a look at technology-supported teams and social loafing. They hypothesized that there are three primary causes of social loafing: diffusion of responsibility, attribution of blame, and dehumanization (Alnuaimi, Robert, & Maruping, 2010). Their experiment supports all three constructs as antecedents for social loafing. Based on their findings of social loafing in technology-supported teams, they hoped their study would encourage other researchers to look at this more closely, as well as managers of technology-supported teams to take this into consideration and make changes accordingly. The participants of the study were undergrad business students. Gender in Social Loafing In studies that have included both males and females as participants, gender seems to play a substantial role in social loafing. In 1983, Kerr found that the male participants of the study were more likely to loaf than the female participants. Karau and Williams (1993) hypothesized that gender played equally a role to social loafing as did culture and individual beliefs about collectivism; their hypothesis was later supported by Kugihara (1999), who showed that females loafed less than males. Stark, Shaw, and Duff (2007) found different levels of loafing between genders in both self and peer-evaluations. Some 64 Journal of Management Policy and Practice vol. 15(1) 2014
Docsity logo



Copyright © 2024 Ladybird Srl - Via Leonardo da Vinci 16, 10126, Torino, Italy - VAT 10816460017 - All rights reserved