Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

Sociocultural Approach to Behaviour: Social Identity Theory and Cultural Influences, Study Guides, Projects, Research of Psychology

Cultural AnthropologySociology of CultureSocial Psychology

The sociocultural approach to behaviour, focusing on social identity theory and its limitations, as well as the development of stereotypes and the role of culture in shaping attitudes and behaviours. The document also discusses ethical considerations in sociocultural research.

What you will learn

  • What role does culture play in shaping attitudes and behaviours?
  • What are the limitations of social identity theory?
  • What ethical considerations should be taken into account in sociocultural research?
  • How does social identity theory explain human behaviour?
  • How do stereotypes develop?

Typology: Study Guides, Projects, Research

2021/2022

Uploaded on 09/27/2022

tarquin
tarquin 🇺🇸

4.3

(14)

40 documents

1 / 34

Toggle sidebar

Related documents


Partial preview of the text

Download Sociocultural Approach to Behaviour: Social Identity Theory and Cultural Influences and more Study Guides, Projects, Research Psychology in PDF only on Docsity! O = oC O © O — a a © o — a) Sas =) O © O o WY _ >) = => © = oO Oo) O) Ge Oo Gc O ~ ” — oO oO Ss s The strength and relevance of social identity theory may depend on individuals’ cultures. People belong to many groups and derive much of their identity from the behaviour of other people within these groups. 88 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour The sociocultural approach to behaviour investigates the role of social and cultural influences in shaping thinking and human behaviour. An increasing body of culturally informed research has made cross-cultural psychology, as well as cultural psychology, a contemporary topic of debate among psychologists working in the field. Theories, concepts, and research studies provide the background fora more nuanced understanding of the variety and complexity of human behaviour. This is important in the modern globalized world where issues related to migration and integration are on the rise. Key figures in the field of modern social psychology have often favoured experimental methods to understand how social context influences behaviours, identities, attitudes, and cognitions. However, by studying acculturation in terms of intergroup relations, we can start to understand that the same processes may be seen in the interaction between cultural groups. Social and cultural psychologists now also use qualitative research methods to get a better understanding of intergroup relations as well as the process of acculturation. The sociocultural approach to behaviour looks at: + the individual and the group (SL and HL) + cultural origins of behaviour (SL and HL) * cultural influences on individual attitudes, identities, and behaviours (SL and HL) + the influence of globalization on individual attitudes, identities, and behaviours (HL only). The individual and the group pic focus To what extent is a person’s behaviour affected by membership of a group? Social identity theor Content focus Evaluate social identity theory (SIT) as an explanation of human behaviour. Tajfel and Turner (1979) proposed social identity theory (SIT) to explain that people's concept of self-identity is derived, at least in part, from the groups they belong to. People belong to many groups, such as their family, school community, workplace and sports teams. SIT proposes that people derive much of their identity from the behaviour of other people within these groups. When people consider themselves a member of one group and not a member of another similar group, their group becomes an ingroup for them. The other, similar groups become their outgroups. Ingroups and outgroups are defined in terms of SIT was largely developed within ‘Western’ contexts. Yuki (2003) suggests SIT is less reliable in explaining behaviour in ‘non-Western’ communities. Yuki (2003) investigated the extent to which SIT is applicable within US and Japanese contexts and found: * loyalty to and identification with their ingroup was greater with American participants than with Japanese participants * — evidence suggests that discrimination against outgroups is more pronounced in individualistic cultures * there is no evidence to support the theory that there is ingroup favouritism. Yuki (2003) concluded that SIT ‘may not accurately represent group behaviours among East Asians’. Stewart et al. (1998) found that Chinese students living in Hong Kong perceived intergroup differentiation to be less important than British students living in Hong Kong did. The British students felt that their group membership was more important and that it generated more positive images of their group than the Chinese students did. Although the results did not contradict SIT, they did suggest that differentiation among the Chinese students is weaker than among the British students. This study focused only on students and so its findings cannot be generalized to the wider Western and Eastern populations. Key study: Howarth (2002) Aim: To examine ‘how the struggle for recognition and esteem permeates everyday experiences in the contexts of young people living in Brixton (South London, UK)’. Procedure: Eight focus groups with a total of 44 teenagers (12-16 years old) separated into friendship groups, and interviews with the three head teachers of Brixton’s secondary schools. Questions and discussion prompts included, ‘Tell me about Brixton. What it is like for you to live here and how do people outside Brixton think about Brixton?’ (Howarth, 2002, p.4). Follow-up interviews were conducted a short while later to clarify and expand on several themes. Findings: The study found that many, but not all, of the young participants held positive perceptions of living in Brixton but believed people who did not live in Brixton did not think highly of it. Conclusion: At least some adolescents develop social and psychological ways to protect themselves from others’ perceptions and judgements about them. The study showed how teenagers’ relationships and the cultures of the institutions to which they belong are able to empower them in respect to their identity. The young participants created a positive identity because of the sense of belonging they felt to their group, i.e. to Brixton residents. Evaluation: Strengths: Interviews can gather rich and varied qualitative information and focus groups have particular strengths, as participants can be prompted to discuss To what extent does the lack of supporting empirical evidence weaken SIT as an explanation of human behaviour? 91 Theories and models are simplified explanations of human behaviour. These theories are often adapted over time as research attempts more detailed explanations. Being simplifications, they come with limitations as well as strengths Always offer balanced discussions of theories and models. Can you identify five things that you do that you have leamed by observing others, perhaps family members or school friends? It may bea way of thinking, talking, or walking, It may be the way you tie your shoelaces or eat your breakfast. SCT's basic hypothesis is that people learn some behaviours by observing others’ behaviours and the consequences of those behaviours 92 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour issues that other group members think of. Interviews can be flexible, allowing the interviewer to delve into themes by developing questions into discussions. The focus group interviews were followed with one-to-one interviews with the heads of the three schools in the area as a means of triangulation, or at least to gather information from a different perspective. The study was gender-balanced and focused on a specific age range that was consistent with its aim and conclusion. Limitations: Qualitative research such as focus group interviews is usually difficult to replicate with the same results. As the essence of the study was about the teenagers’ perceptions of their ingroup, interviewing the participants in small friendship-based focus groups rather than conducting individual interviews raises the possibility of peer pressure compromising the authenticity of the teenagers’ responses. Evaluate social cognitive theory (SCT) as an explanation of human behaviour. Social cognitive theory (SCT) is a learning theory: it attempts to explain how people learn new behaviours. SCT suggests behaviour is acquired through observation or imitation of others. The theory proposes that when a learner observes another person's behaviour and sees the consequences of that behaviour (rewards or punishments), they remember and use this information with respect to their own behaviour. The model whose behaviour is mimicked or learned can be real (e.g. a family member, school teacher, or sports player) or fictional (e.g. a movie or television character). Bandura et al. (1961) argued that people's behaviour is caused by personal, behavioural, and environmental influences. Whether a learner reproduces a behaviour that has been modelled or demonstrated is influenced by: * _ personal factors: the extent to which the learner has low or high self-efficacy (their belief that they will succeed) * behavioural factors: the extent to which the learner is rewarded after performing the behaviour correctly * environmental factors: external barriers or supports that affect the learner's ability to reproduce the behaviour. 13.2.1 Social cognitive theory and self-efficacy Self-efficacy refers to a person’s perception of the likelihood that they will succeed. It relates to SCT in that if learners do not think they can succeed, they are less likely to try to replicate a model's behaviour. White et al. (2012) tested whether SCT could affect physical activity participation in middle-aged and older adults. Participants’ self-efficacy level, ability limitations, goals, outcome expectations, and physical activity were measured at the beginning and end ofan 18-month period. Self-efficacy was thought to influence the participants’ level of physical activity directly, but also indirectly by affecting the participants’ goals and expectations. The researchers found that participants with higher levels of self-efficacy participated in greater levels of physical activity and had fewer disability limitations impeding their participation. SCT has been applied to health psychology to help people stop cigarette smoking by increasing smoker's self-efficacy (their belief that they will be able to quit). Smokers were shown ways of experiencing success through role-playing and imagination. Smokers were also shown models who had successfully quit smoking, i.e. behaviour they could mimic. This method was effective in helping smokers to stop smoking. Stajkovic and Luthans (1979) clarified self-efficacy in the workplace by stating that unless employees believe they can bring together required behavioural, cognitive, and motivational resources needed to execute a task, they will most likely focus on the most difficult aspects of a task and apply insufficient effort (Stajkovic et al., 1979). Ahmed and Sands (2009) aimed to determine if more mothers of pre-term infants breastfed their babies after a SCT-influenced breastfeeding education programme. Sixty Egyptian mothers were randomly assigned to groups: (1) participating in the programme, or (2) routine care. The education programme was made up of SCT strategies: (1) showing role models who breastfed correctly to improve the participants’ self-efficacy; (2) weekly check-ups over the course of three months to reinforce the participants’ breastfeeding skills; and (3) a self-report checklist for the participants to encourage the breastfeeding. The study found that mothers in the education programme showed significant improvement in breastfeeding and were more likely to only breastfeed their babies, i.e. no bottle feeding. These mothers also had significantly fewer breastfeeding issues than the mothers who were not in the education programme. 13.2.2 Social cognitive theory and aggression Bandura et al. (1961) aimed to find out when and why children display aggressive behaviours. They conducted a study where adults demonstrated verbal and physical aggression toward an inflatable doll (called a Bobo doll) in the presence of preschool- aged children, many of whom subsequently reproduced the aggressive behaviour. CES andura et al. (196 Aim: To find out why and when children display aggressive behaviours. Procedure: An experiment was conducted with 72 children (36 boys and 36 girls) aged 3-6, all of whom were enrolled in Stanford University’s day-care programme. While the participants (children) played with books and stickers, an aggressive adult model (sometimes a male model, sometimes a female model) played briefly but shortly afterwards spent the rest of the time behaving aggressively (both physically and verbally) toward the Bobo doll, ina standardized way that would be used with each of the children. At the same time another (adult) model played in a subdued, non-aggressive way. 93 To what extent does television perpetuate, or even create, stereotypes? Think about television programmes that are watched in countries different to the country they were created in. To what extent did your responses in the previous activity include stereotypes of people living in Pakistan, Germany, and Russia? Research successful entrepreneurs or sportspeople and look for examples of correspondence bias. An illusory correlation is a simple error of association. It is the incorrect conclusion that two events that occur at the same time are related, 96 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour Evaluation: The study was based on verbal reports from the participants, so the responses required little effort. The study only aimed to investigate stereotypes held by US students, so the study’s conclusion is of limited value with regards to stereotypes held by wider populations. The study does not explain why the stereotypes exists, just that they do. The study was conducted in 1933, ata time when prejudices were strongly held and widespread, so the study's findings simply gave confirmation to widely known information. The study was conducted on participants at a prestigious university, so itis likely that the participants were mostly young, Caucasian males from a relatively upper socioeconomic sector of US society, meaning the conclusion cannot be generalized to wider populations. There is no evidence of attempting to correct the participants’ stereotypes. Ina follow-up study, Gilbert (1951) found the same stereotypes still existed but the extent of consensus was lower. Devine and Elliott (1995) modified Katz and Braley’s (1933) attribute list with new attributes, such as ostentatious and pleasure-seeking. There was low consensus for Katz and Braley’s attributes, and high consensus for the new ones, suggesting Katz and Braley’s attribute list may be outdated and current beliefs are less negative. An inherent weakness of self-report questionnaires is that participants may not report accurately. Once stereotypes have been formed they tend to persist because of correspondence bias, the illusory correlation, upbringing, and ingroup and outgroup relations. 13.3.2 Correspondence bias and stereotype formation Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation. Correspondence bias is the tendency to over-attribute a person's behaviour to their personality (dispositional) factors, and to under-attribute the extent to which external (situational) factors caused the behaviour. Nier and Gaertner (2012) found that people who display correspondence bias tend to stereotype high-status groups as competent and low-status groups as incompetent. Participants who scored highly on correspondence bias assessment stereotyped the poor, women, and a fictitious group of low-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent. The same participants stereotyped the rich, men, and the fictitious group of high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. After controlling for other variables, correspondence bias was the most significant predictor of stereotyping by the participants. 13.3.3 Illusory correlation and stereotype formation An illusory correlation is a simple error of association: the incorrect conclusion that two events that occur at the same time are related. For example, if someone breaks amirror and the same day breaks their arm, they may correlate the two events and conclude that the broken mirror caused the broken arm, when in fact the two events are unrelated. Unusual events are distinctive and so people notice them more than mundane, normal events. The increased attention leads to stronger or more effective encoding, which strengthens the perception that the events are associated. In an intergroup context, illusory correlations cause people to wrongly attribute uncommon behaviours to minority groups or outgroups. Hamilton and Gifford (1976) tested the extent to which illusory correlation leads to stereotype formation. The study found illusory correlation is stronger when the infrequent and distinctive information is negative. CES) (1976) Aim: To investigate the illusory correlation of group size and negative behaviour. Hamilton and Giff Procedure: Researchers asked 114 university students from the US to read descriptions and then make conclusions about two made-up groups, Group A and Group B. The descriptions were based on a number of positive and negative behaviours. Group A, the larger group with 26 members, performed 18 positive and 8 negative behaviours. Group B, the smaller group with 13 members, performed 9 positive and 4 negative behaviours. Number of members in the group | 26 13 Number of positive behaviours 18 9 Number of negative behaviours 8 4 Findings: Although there was no correlation between group membership and the types of behaviours exhibited by the groups (the proportion of negative and positive behaviours was the same for both groups) the participants did seem to make an illusory correlation because more of the undesirable/negative behaviours were attributed to the minority Group B, than the majority Group A. Conclusion: The findings support the theory that distinctive information draws attention. The number of Group B members and negative behaviours are both numerically fewer than those of Group A and therefore more distinctive than Group A’s. The participants’ responses were not logically correct and so showed an illusory correlation. Evaluation: The study concludes that the illusory correlation exists because people's attention is drawn to distinctive or unusual phenomena, suchas the small number of people in Group B. This is the explanation given for some people making an illusory correlation relating to minority groups and their behaviour, such as Aboriginal Australians and alcohol abuse. This illusory correlation could also be called a failure of understanding simple statistics. The study was conducted under partially controlled conditions, but the most significant variable, the participants’ level of understanding of mathematics and statistics, is not considered. McConnell et al. (1994) found that people formed stereotypes based on information that was considered distinctive at the time of judgement rather than when the information was first encountered. Consistent with Bartlett's (1932) cognitive concept of ‘effort after meaning’, when a person decides that previously non-distinctive information already encoded into memory is distinctive, that information is re-encoded as if it were distinctive when it was first noticed. To what extent are superstitions formed because of illusory correlations? 97 People acquire or develop some stereotypes from their environmental experiences, ie. from parents, the media, peers, and teachers, as well as direct contact with the outgroup members. If stereotype formation isa normal human behaviour, are discriminatory practices such as ageism, sexism, and racism also normal human behaviour? To what extent do the cultural aspects of a study's participant sample hinder the ability to generalize that study's results? Can studies of human behaviour conducted more than ten years ago apply to behaviour now? To what extent can a study of young children in one country or culture conclude that all people form stereotypes from their environment? 98 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour 13.3.4 Upbringing and stereotype formation Stereotypes may also be the consequence of a person's upbringing. Some stereotypes may develop in early childhood because they are presented to children by parents, teachers, friends, and the media. Bar-Tal (1996) investigated the role of upbringing in the formation of a stereotype of Arabs in Jewish children in Israel. Two hundred and fourteen children (102 boys and 112 girls, aged 2-6 years) from two socioeconomically different Tel-Aviv neighbourhoods were individually shown a photograph of an Arab man wearing the traditional kaffia. Each child was then asked to rate him against four traits (good/bad, dirty/clean, handsome/ugly, and weak/strong). The results showed that almost all the children had already developed a negative stereotype of Arabs. The study concluded that children acquire or develop some stereotypes from their environmental experiences, i.e. from parents, media, peers, and teachers, as well as direct contact with the outgroup members. 13.3.5 Ingroup and outgroup relations and stereotype formation The ingroup-outgroup explanation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes are formed and shared because group members are motivated to strengthen their perceived similarities with their ingroup and strengthen their perceived differences with their outgroups. This means the stereotypes are a consequence, not a cause, of intergroup relationships. In the same way that research shows that intergroup problems based on perceived ingroup-outgroup differences can be overcome by merging the groups and initiating contact and communication between group members, stereotypes can also be overcome. HMI... ANI GROVES AND OUTERDURS, VERY INTERESTING, NoAM CHOMSKY SKID WE'LL LEARN MoRE ABOUT Mouse BEHAVoue FROM NovELS TRAN SCENTIFIC PSYCHPLOGY, / { THK He's RIGHT. Gdoowiy. RESEARCHING AN EXTENDED ESSAY ON in GRDUPS ANID OVTGROUPS ... OR MAYBE iT WILL BE ON STEREOTYPES. The FAE is altered in the Indian context to the extent that dispositional factors tend to be more interpersonal rather than purely personal (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). For example, Indian people tend to understate the role of situational factors such as the weather in their successes and overstate the role of factors such as the effect of their friends and family members as well as their own dispositional factors (Markus and Kitayama, 1991). These studies showed that FAE is less of a universal human behaviour than previously thought, and that it is instead affected by a person's culture. | COULD HAVE BEEN RicH. i WAS CLEVER. | INVENTED THAT. | MADE THE PRoTeTypE. AND THEN WHAT? THE \ GovERNMENT REFuseD My PATENT AND A HUMAN STOLE iT. 1 CouLD HAVE \ BEEN FAMouS. — Goodwin GRIMACING MouSE DiScoVERS FAE. 14.1.3 Counting and arithmetic Saxe (2015) demonstrated that the cognitive processes of counting and arithmetic are affected by culture. For example, the body-part counting system used by the Oksapmin people in Papua New Guinea is the 27-number equivalent of the ‘Western’ ten-number counting system based on counting with fingers and thumbs (Saxe, 2015). Reed and Lave (1979) conducted a natural experiment, which was supplemented with observations and interviews, involving tailors from the Vai and Gola tribes in Monrovia (Liberia) to investigate the effect of culture on arithmetic thinking. The study found that the tailors who used the Vai/Gola numerical system had a very different cognitive process for counting and conducting arithmetic problem solving. Beller and Bender (2008) used examples from Melanesian and Polynesian culture to show the cultural origins of their unique arithmetic problem-solving skill: adding and subtracting the large numbers required by large-scale and long-distance trading systems. In the High Fijian language, a different word is used to count 100 (bolu) canoes and 100 (koro) coconuts. In Managreva (French Polynesia) different number sequences are used for counting tools, breadfruit, and octopus. (Beller and Bender, 2008). 101 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour Oksapmin 27-body-part counting system The Oksapmin 27-body-part counting system uses body parts’ names, starting with the thumb of one hand as 1, moving around the fingers to 5, up the arm and to the shoulder, 10, up the neck and across the ears, eyes, nose and down to the neck, 17, and then down the other arm, hand, and to the other thumb, 27. When shown a group of five piles of ten sticks, teachers will teach children that this is little finger (representing 5) followed by a shoulder (representing 10) (Saxe, 2015). 15, Figure 14.1 The Oksapmin 27-body-part counting system IFyou know people from other countries, ‘or perhaps from earlier generations, ask them to explain the way they add, subtract, multiply, or divide. Key study: Reed and Lave (1979) Aim: To investigate the role of culture on counting and arithmetic problem solving. Procedure: The research was based on participant observation and informal interviews with 140 males of the Vai and Gola tribes working as tailors in shops of Tailors’ Alley in Monrovia, Liberia. The interviews covered apprentices’ personal history, family background, and beliefs about teaching and learning. Some tasks were aimed at understanding arithmetic skills. Tailors used either the apprenticed Vai/Gola arithmetic system or the school-taught Western arithmetic system to solve a series of arithmetic problems. The Vai/Gola arithmetic is a spoken numeration system (i.e. it’s not written) and seldom uses numbers larger than 20. Vai/Gola arithmetic operations are done either ‘in the head’ or by manipulating sets of markers. Findings: The study demonstrated consistent differences in the methods employed by tailors who had learned arithmetic in the tailor shop as apprentices and in school. Problem-solving procedures were closely related to the tailors’ culture- dependent learning experiences. The traditionally taught (apprenticed) Vai/Gola tailors used counters, such as pebbles, or marks on paper, to count and conduct arithmetic problem solving, as well as mental arithmetic problem solving. The tailors trained in the Western school system used algorithmic manipulations learnt in school and strategies using number names. Using data based on arithmetic problem solving, observations and recordings of verbal protocols, and interviews about the apprenticeship and schooling experiences, the study showed that the ‘folk arithmetic system’ was as systematic as the Western, school-taught system. 102 There was strong evidence to show that all except the least experienced apprentices had a clear understanding of the general idea of arithmetic problem solving consistent with their arithmetic system. Conclusion: The study found that there is an arithmetic problem-solving system unique to the Vai and Gola tribes and that this is used by tailors trained through traditional apprenticeship methods, i.e. not in school, and that there is another, different system used by those with five or more years of schooling. The tailors who used the Vai/Gola numerical system had a very different cognitive process for counting and conducting arithmetic problem solving. Instead of re-coding quantities as words or numerals, apprentices using the Vai/Gola counting system simply used pebbles or counters, one for each unit counted. Evaluation: The study has high ecological validity as it was conducted in context. The tailors were observed while they were using their arithmetic systems, not ina laboratory setting. This meant that other variables were not controlled. Interviews and experiments supported the researchers’ numerous observations. 6,7, 7, i'VE NEVER REALLY UNDERSTOOD 4, Godwin — 14.1.4 Surface and deep culture Surface culture refers to the obvious or readily discernible differences between a person's indigenous and host countries, for example language, gestures, diet, clothing, and interpersonal behaviour. Deep culture refers to more profound cultural norms; those that are considerably less obvious and therefore less accessible to newcomers, such as social hierarchies, interpretations of dignity and respect, religion, and humour. Individuals who have a low level of acculturation with respect to a dominant culture are more likely to retain the cultural values of their indigenous community and therefore behave less in response to their new culture’s norms (Betancourt and Lopez, 1993). Investigate counting systems such as the Oksapmin 27-body-part ‘counting system and the Vai/Gola system. To what extent does culture affect people's system of counting and arithmetic? 103 To what extent might the concept of cultural dimensions contribute to stereotypes of nationalities or cultures? Do you think cultural dimensions might change ‘over time, perhaps due to globalization or technology use? A person's values are described as cultural dimensions and are measured or quantified on acontinuum, i.e. to what extent is a particular value a strong force in a person's decisions about behaviour. Culture is not the same as ethnicity or nationality. Use specific language to be clear about this, for example, ‘the way children of the Samoan culture learn the nuances of their language is affected by the way they are“encultured’, ie by the way they acquire the norms and expectations of their Samoan culture’ 106 TOK, Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour These five orientations combine to create a set of cultural dimensions, each of which can be considered a continuum on which a person can be placed in answer to the question, ‘To what extent do you value this orientation in your daily life decisions?” 14.2.2 Cultural dimensions Hofstede (1984) proposed four cultural dimensions: * power distance * — individualism * masculinity * uncertainty avoidance. Power distance refers to the extent to which less powerful individuals accept inequality as the norm. Inequality exists in all cultures, but individuals’ acceptance of it varies across cultures. Individualist societies are those in which people place the greatest focus on themselves and their immediate family and friends while collectivist societies are made up of people who place greater value on wider, extended family groups from which they have great difficulty separating themselves. These extended families can be considered an individual's ‘ingroup’. The masculinity dimension refers to individuals’ expectation that men will be assertive, ambitious, and competitive and to respect things that are big, strong, and fast. Masculine cultures expect women to care for children and others who cannot care for themselves, while feminine cultures expect men and women to be equally ambitious and caring. Uncertainty avoidance, according to Hofstede (1984), refers to the extent to which individuals are tolerant of uncertainty. Cultures with a strong tolerance of uncertainty are active, emotional and tolerant while those with a low tolerance of uncertainty are less aggressive, more accepting of personal risk, and relatively intolerant. Cultural influences on individual attitudes, identity, and behaviours Topic focus Discuss cultural influences on individual attitudes, identity, and behaviour. 5.1 Enculturation Content focus Using one or more examples, discuss the role enculturation plays in one behaviour. Enculturation is the process of acquiring a culture’s norms. At its simplest level, it is the way people learn the ‘rules’ of their culture or what is expected by a group of its members. For example, learning the unwritten rules when starting at a new school, or how to behave appropriately when joining a new workplace, sports team, or group of friends is done through enculturation. People teach their group's norms and expected behaviour, i.e. their group’s culture, tonew members of the group. People can learn from each other because of social cognition. People take others’ perspectives to an extent that allows them to experience the group’s behaviour (Tomasello et al., 1993). Atthe simplest level, enculturation occurs through direct instruction. Parents teach their children the appropriate way to behave by telling them what is right and wrong, for example they tell them to speak quietly in restaurants. New workers to an organization are instructed by supervisors during orientation about how and when to carry out their assigned duties, how to dress, and which communication protocols to follow. Teaching culture this way occurs because it is direct, deliberate, and effective. Enculturation can also occur through social learning, i.e. learning by an individual who is influenced by the social environment (Bandura et al., 1961). This influence may be minimal or superficial, for example parents or teachers may simply give young children objects such as a ball, which they explore or play with to discover new behaviour. In such cases the social environment only exposes the culture's new member to the object; the actual learning process is individual. New students often learn the social rules of their new school (expected norms such as popular hairstyles), in-class behaviours (such as hand raising), and the accepted way to greet classmates and teachers by observing other students’ behaviour. This is possible because of social cognition. People can also learn cultural expectations through cultural learning. New group members do not just observe and then mimic others’ behaviour (as with social cognition), they try to see a situation the way other group members see it. This learning is social; the new group member tries to learn the group's norms by empathizing with or imagining another person's perspective or point of view. 15.1.1 Enculturation’s effect on language Enculturation affects the way one learns language. Pinker (1994) proposes children begin life being aware, to some extent, that the sounds their mother produces are elements of a language and that those sounds have meaning, as compared to sounds such as a door closing, which does not have meaning. Others, however, believe that language is a cognitive consequence of enculturation. Tomasello and Rakoczy (2003) propose that while children’s understanding of language occurs ontogenetically in all cultural settings at about 1 year old, the understanding of beliefs occurs some years later, and at different ages in different cultural settings. There is strong evidence that participating in language-based communication with other people is a necessary condition for a child's development (Tomasello and Rakoczy, 2003). Ochs (1982) showed that Samoan culture places great emphasis on learning by observation and so the cognitive process of language acquisition by Samoan children is affected significantly by their families’ cultures. For example, as Samoan children Enculturation is the process of acquiring or learning what is, expected by a group of its members. Discuss with three or four ‘others in your class the process of enculturation when a new student starts in your school Write down ten ‘rules’ or expectations imposed by the school, some of which are formal and overt (known to all) and some which are covert (known only to the students). How do students learn these rules or expectations of behaving in the school? How did you learn the rules of your school? What were the five most important rules that you were not told about? 107 108 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour learn to become members of their society or culture, they also learn to become competent users of their language, meaning enculturation and language acquisition occur simultaneously. A significant part of their enculturation is the way they use their language. Key study: Ochs (1982) Aim: To investigate language development and language socialization in traditional Samoan households. Procedure: A longitudinal case study observed 23 children (under 6 years old) from different households every five weeks over a period of ten months. Recordings were transcribed by the children’s households and checked by the researchers. Findings: The study found that language instruction is conducted by a child's caregiver, who is most often an older sibling, and that language instruction is characterized by three culture-laden features: (1) decentring, (2) a lack of expansion by caregivers, and (3) elicited imitation. Higher status people, such as parents, do not lower their perspective to communicate with lower status people, such as children. Conclusion: Traditional Samoan culture places great emphasis on learning by observation and so the cognitive process of language acquisition by Samoan children is significantly affected by their families’ and community's cultures and by the process of learning that culture, i.e. enculturation. The way the children’s caregivers use language while being observed by the children affects the way the children learn about their culture’s norms. This means that child—parent communication is often ‘child-to-parent-to-caregiver-to-child’ so that children observe the cultural status of their parent but observe the language of their caregiver, most likely a slightly older sibling. Within Samoan culture, people believe they have little control over their own actions. For example, they tend to say ‘legs walk’, ‘hands write’, ‘mouths eat’, and ‘the head is dizzy’, and they have no vocabulary for ‘individual’ or ‘personality’ (Ochs, 1982). This means that Samoans are less inclined to communicate with their children. Children are not treated as socially responsive beings, i.e. not cooperative, and they are not considered to be in control of themselves and their actions. The vocal utterances of Samoan children are treated as sounds rather than the beginnings of language. The social stratification of Samoan society, as well as the emphasis Samoan society places on observational learning, determines the ways children learn and use their language (Ochs 1982). Higher status Samoans, including those caring for children, are not expected to adjust their perspective to that of lower status people. Higher status Samoans are allowed to express their opinions while lower status Samoans are expected to speak about the collective rather than themselves. The children learn their language because they are cared for by a broad range of relatives; initially their mother and then by male and female grandparents and older siblings. Often several older siblings are responsible for caring for and teaching younger children. Samoan society is status-oriented and those of high status are not involved in child-rearing. Showed a strong identity with their own culture and a strong proficiency and usage in their own language as well as maintaining strong relationships with those from their own culture. Supported the separation attitude. Support for family values was high. Remained embedded in their own culture and demonstrated little involvement in their host country’s culture. Demonstrated a relatively strong orientation toward their new culture. Identified with their host country and demonstrated low identity with respect to their original culture Proficient in the host country’s language and used it most often. Friendships/peer contacts were mostly with people from their host country. Demonstrated relatively low support for family obligations In general, retained little of their own culture and their original identity. Reported high ability and usage of their own language, but low identity with regards to their original culture. Reported low proficiency in the language of their host country and low identity with respect to their host country. Had few peer contacts outside their own cultural group. Appeared to want to be part of the larger society but lacked the language and social skills or opportunities to make contacts. Conclusion: Involvement and engagement in both their host country’s and their ethnic cultures are associated with better adaptation for immigrant youth than a preference for either their original or their new culture alone. Integration had a positive effect on the immigrants’ adaptation. Adolescents’ attitude and engagement with respect to their own cultural group is more important for their psychological well-being than their sociocultural adaptation. Evaluation: The study was conducted with a very large number of participants in 13 host countries. The study was based on self-report questionnaires that are subject to demand characteristics and a possible lack of honesty. The host countries in the study (Australia, Canada, Finland, France, Germany, Israel, Netherlands, New Zealand, Norway, Portugal, Sweden, UK and USA) are all loosely described as Judeo-Christian. Berry et al. (2006) found that participants in the study who also identified with Judeo-Christian belief systems or with Eastern religions integrated better than those who did not. Participants from a Muslim background integrated least well although this may have been distorted by the large number of Turkish immigrants to Europe who were expected to return to Turkey and therefore were less inclined to integrate with their host country’s culture. To what extent do the circumstances prompting emigration froma country affect the extent to which an immigrant adapts and assimilates into a new culture? To what extent does acculturation promote or overcome nationality based stereotypes? Try to meet someone who came into your cultural group some time ago and discuss with them the process they went through to adapt/assimilate. (This may not necessarily be an immigrant to your country.) What did they find most difficult about their acculturation process? What was not difficult? Did they have to learn a new language? How do they feel they have changed as a result of their acculturation? The text refers to geographical mobility as a motivation for acculturation, To what extent can acculturation theory be applied to other situations, such as moving to a new sports club? 111 Be clear to distinguish between research methods, such as natural experiments, and data- collection techniques, such as self-report questionnaires. List ten naturally occurring phenomena in humans that could be used as the IV ina quasi-experiment. A correlation study focuses on variables to determine if they are co-variables, ie. if they co-occur. The experimental method tests for a cause-effect link between co-variables. 112 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour Research methods: sociocultural approach Learning focus Explain the contribution of one research method to the sociocultural approach to understanding human behaviour. Social and cultural psychologists use both qualitative and quantitative research methods to gain an understanding of intergroup relations as well as the process of acculturation. 6.1 Quasi-experiments A quasi-experiment studies variables that are inherent to the participants, for example the participants’ height, handedness, nationality or ethnicity. This means the participants are not allocated by the experimenter to the Independent variable’s (IV) different conditions. Typically, cross-cultural studies are quasi-experiments, using participants’ cultures as the IV and a behaviour as the dependent variable (DV). Quasi-experiments tend to be conducted outside ‘laboratory conditions’ and so other variables are less likely to be controlled. This means the studies are difficult to replicate. Cross-cultural studies can be used to determine whether a behaviour is unique to some cultures, i.e. culture-bound, or whether it is cross-cultural, i.e. observed in all cultures. Yuki (2003) conducted a cross-cultural study using quasi-experiments to investigate the extent to which SIT is applicable within US and Japanese contexts, i.e. to determine whether it is a culture-bound or cross-cultural phenomenon. In this study the IV is the participants’ culture (Japanese or American). The DV is the extent to which SIT applies to each context. Questionnaires were used to gather data relating to the participants’ attitudes toward groups and their behaviour within groups. The study found (1) loyalty to and identification with their ingroup was greater with American participants than with Japanese participants; (2) evidence suggests that discrimination against outgroups is more pronounced in individualistic cultures; and (3) there is no evidence to support the theory that there is ingroup favouritism. Yuki (2003) concluded that SIT ‘may not accurately represent group behaviours among East Asians’ and therefore is not a cross- cultural phenomenon. 16.2 Correlation study using a self-report questionnaire A correlation study focuses on variables to determine if they are co-variables, i.e. if they co-occur. Correlation means two variables occur together or are related. Berry et al. (2006) focused on the variables of acculturation strategies and the success of adaptation and assimilation to determine a correlation. The study found that acculturation strategies that included involvement and engagement with the host culture (such as high language proficiency and peer contact with those from the host culture) was a co-variable with successful adaptation and assimilation. The study did not show a cause-effect link between the variables. Further study, using the experimental method, is needed to test for a cause-effect link between variables. Self-report questionnaires are a simple, relatively quick, and inexpensive way to collect data and are easy to replicate. Questionnaires, however, rely on participants’ honesty. Participants may unwittingly give inaccurate responses out of a desire to please the researchers, or to appear to be better than they really are. The reliability of questionnaires is dependent on participants’ ability to understand the questions. Berry et al. (2006) used responses to self-report questionnaires to gather data relating to acculturation despite a significant number of participants not having strong language proficiency. 6.3 Emic and etic approaches to research Cultures can be studied by an insider or by an outsider. When research is conducted by an insider, it is considered emic. In emic studies, the researcher has first-hand experience, often acquired over a sustained period. The disadvantage is that the researcher lacks a professional distance, the necessary separation to be objective about the research. When conducted by an insider, the research is emic and when studied by an outsider itis etic. List some strengths and weaknesses of both etic and emic research into people's attitudes toward time. When research is conducted by an outsider, it is considered etic. Ochs (1982) studied the enculturation process of Samoan children’s language acquisition using an etic approach. The behaviour was observed by outsiders, i.e. non-Samoan researchers lived within the culture, but were of another culture. Howarth’s (2002) study of teenagers living in Brixton was also etic, because the researcher was an outsider. The universalist approach assumes there are shared psychological processes in all human behaviour (including the acculturation process). The relativist approach assumes cultural groups’ psychological processes are different and so the groups cannot be compared Malhotra et al. (1996) stated that the emic approach examines phenomena from within, investigates just one culture, and the criteria adopted are relative; while the etic approach examines phenomena from outside the system, investigates many cultures, and the criteria adopted are universal. The universalist approach assumes there are psychological processes shared in all human cultures, for example the language acquisition process. By contrast, the relativist approach assumes cultural groups’ psychological processes, for example the perception of beauty and intelligence, are different, and so they cannot be compared. Yuki (2003) examined the extent to which social identity theory is cross-cultural, adopting an emic approach when studying Japanese participants and an etic approach when studying American participants. 1 v4 Ethical considerations: sociocultural approach Learning focus Show that ethical considerations are the right thing to do for everyone involved in research, supporting your claims with examples of good or bad practice To what extent are ethical considerations necessary in the investigation of the sociocultural approach to understanding human behaviour? 17.1 Studies involving child A fundamental rule of most ethical systems is to do no harm. Bandura et al. (1961) was unethical because it exposed young children to aggressive behaviour and taught 113 Present balanced discussions and arguments. Be careful to avoid unwarranted generalizations and stereotypes. Discuss globalization with your classmates. The word is used frequently in many school subjects, but what does it mean? What causes it? Is it a good thing? Or a bad thing? How does it manifestin you and your classmates lives? Books, theatre, cinema, and television have all played a significant role in developing people's global identities because they have exposed people to the rest of the world, although mostly to the dominant Western world, Create an interactive wall chart that invites passers-by to write comments on it about what they value, and why. You should begin with some examples such as, ‘Ivalue honesty because it means | can trust and rely on others, or 'I value my family and friends because. After one week, replace the wall chart with a summary of the values and value statements People who acculturate with bicultural identity show the best psychological adaptation. 116 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour The influence of globalization on individual behaviour (HL) General focus Discuss the influence of globalization on individual behaviour. 8.1 Globalization may influence cult Learning focus Discuss how globalization may influence behaviour. Arnett (2002) states that the most salient psychological consequence of globalization is the effect on people's identity, ie. how people perceive themselves with respect to the social environment in which they live. Two aspects of identity are affected by globalization: (1) because of globalization many people in the world develop a bicultural identity — part of their identity comes from their local or indigenous culture and another part comes from their relationship with the global culture; and (2) there are people who purposefully form themselves into self-selected cultures that are ‘pure of infection’ from the global culture (Arnett, 2002). 18.1.1 Bicultural identities Young people develop a global identity that gives them a sense of belonging to a worldwide culture as well as their local identity. Books, theatre, cinema, and television have all played a significant role in developing people's global identities because they have exposed people to the rest of the world, although mostly to the dominant Western world. The internet is likely to play an even more important role in people's global identity, because it allows direct, instant, and two-way communication with other people anywhere in the world. While most people develop a global identity that allows them to interact with people from all over the world, people still develop their local identities based on their local culture; and this is their ‘default’ identity (Arnett, 2002). A bicultural identity is found among many young ‘middle-class’ Indians; young, Western-educated Indians. While they are active participants in the globalized world, they still generally prefer an arranged marriage and expect to care for their elderly parents, as is the Indian tradition (Verma and Saraswathi, 2002). These young, globalized people retain their traditional Indian identity while also developing a global identity. Immigration promotes globalization (Hermans and Kempen, 1998). The identities of immigrants and, toa lesser extent, those from the host country’s culture incorporate aspects of each other's culture. Berry (1997) found that people who acculturate with bicultural identity show the best psychological adaptation. 18.1.2 Self-selected cultures Core values of the global culture are: individual/personal rights; the freedom of choice for individuals; open-mindedness to change, and tolerance of differences. These values are prevalent and dominant in the global culture because they are the generally held values of people within the cultures that power globalization, i.e. the wealthy and ‘the West’. For most people these values are appealing, but some people choose to reject them and join a culture that provides a more personalized identity, as they refuse to be one of the global crowd. The global culture is, in general, secular and often the self-selected cultures of people who shun the global culture have a religious basis. For example, when some women who grew up in secular Jewish homes in the US reached adulthood they decided that the secular values their families raised them with provided an inadequate foundation for their lives and they embraced Orthodox Judaism (Arnett, 2002). Despite the strict gender roles and limitations on women, Orthodox Judaism gave the women the structure and meaning of Orthodox Jewish theology, and a sense of belonging to an enduring tradition (Arnett, 2002). Similarly, Samoans have recently revived their practice of tattooing adolescent males’ bodies; traditionally, this was considered essential for achieving adult status, but now seems to be a part of an attempt to resist the loss of their indigenous culture (Arnett, 2002). The revival of interest in indigenous languages is also likely to be a consequence of people rejecting, at least in part, the global culture and the strengthening of one’s local culture and therefore local culture- based identity. 18.2 The effect of the interaction of local and global influences on behaviour Learning focus Discuss the interaction of local and global influences on behaviour. Arnett (2002) highlights another psychological consequence of globalization: the effect of the interaction of local and global influences on behaviour. This shows in two ways: (1) the pervasiveness of identity confusion among young people who find themselves identifying with neither their local culture nor the global culture; and (2) people's pre-adulthood discovery process regarding their identity in work and in relationships increases in duration, going beyond the adolescent period (10-18 years old) into the emerging adulthood period (18-25 years old). Berry (1997) proposed the term ‘acculturative stress’ to describe the conflict between one's original culture and a new culture, and the effect they have on a person's identity. The stress is greatest when the values of the indigenous culture are incompatible with those of the global culture. This acculturative stress contributes to identity confusion, so the greater the acculturative stress the greater the identity confusion (Berry, 1997). To what extent do students in Aotearoa/ New Zealand feel their indigenous identity is being protected from the global culture through the teaching and learning of te reo Maori (the Maori language)? Find and interview someone who remembers life before the internet. Interview them to determine what behaviours may have changed for this person since the internets creation, The terms global and local are relative. Local, for example, can refer to family, region, country, as well as social groups. Can you describe two cultures with values that are compatible? And then two cultures with values that are incompatible? 117 Ahybrid identity is an identity that forms on the basis of the local and global culture. 118 Socioculturalapproachtounderstandingbehaviour 18.2.1 Identity hybridisation Having a bicultural identity means that a person adopts a local identity as well as a distinct and separate global identity. In some cases, globalization alters a person's indigenous beliefs and values to the extent that the local and global cultures are blended, generating a hybrid identity (Arnett, 2002). As globalization causes local cultures to develop, most people’s identities also adapt to either become a bicultural ora hybrid identity, which allows them to live in their indigenous culture as well as engage with their global culture (Arnett, 2002). 18.2.2 Identity confusion Some people, however, experience identity confusion because they lose contact with their indigenous culture but are also unable or unwilling to engage with the global culture. Berry (1997) identified marginalization: an acculturation pattern in which people have little interest or an inability to maintain their indigenous culture, but also reject the global culture. Globalization seems to have the greatest effect on young people's identity. Where a child lives is now less important than it was in the past, with respect to what he or she understands of the world, because of the extent to which young people learn the global culture through television, cinema, and the internet (Arnett, 2002). Globalization leads to culture shedding, as the global culture affects indigenous cultures. For example, traditionally paternal or patriarchal cultures that are exposed to ‘Western’ family structures are becoming more egalitarian as a consequence of exposure to the global culture through television, cinema, and the internet (Nsamenang, 2002). The people most likely to suffer identity confusion because of globalization are those living in cultures that have the greatest cultural distance between them and the global culture. Some cultures have experienced an increase in social problems, especially in young people, likely to be due to identity confusion arising from globalization. Using interviews with young people, supported by data from police and social workers, Delafosse et al. (1993) studied changes in problems among young people aged 16-20 years in Céte d'Ivoire from 1980 to 1991 and found ‘an increase over this period in suicide, drug abuse, armed aggression, and male and female prostitution’. Researchers attributed these behaviours to globalization. 18.2.3 Postponed adulthood Another worldwide psychological consequence of globalization is the timing of the transition into adulthood, with more young people choosing to postpone work, marriage, and parenthood. This is occurring in every part of the world because of a global trend of people spending more time in education, meaning the transition into adulthood roles is delayed (Arnett, 2000). As traditional family structures change through exposure to the global culture on television, cinema, books, magazines, and the internet, all of which are dominated by ‘Western values’, young people tend to take more control over their lives and, although not universal, the median ages for the transition to adulthood is rising rapidly in all countries (Arnett, 2000).
Docsity logo



Copyright © 2024 Ladybird Srl - Via Leonardo da Vinci 16, 10126, Torino, Italy - VAT 10816460017 - All rights reserved