Docsity
Docsity

Prepara tus exámenes
Prepara tus exámenes

Prepara tus exámenes y mejora tus resultados gracias a la gran cantidad de recursos disponibles en Docsity


Consigue puntos base para descargar
Consigue puntos base para descargar

Gana puntos ayudando a otros estudiantes o consíguelos activando un Plan Premium


Orientación Universidad
Orientación Universidad

Reflexive Empathy and Imitation: Social Bonds and Reproductive Success in Baboons - Prof. , Apuntes de Etología

The adaptive nature of skills related to empathy and imitation in forming strong, enduring social bonds among baboons. Evidence from both humans and baboons, highlighting the crucial role of mental state attributions and affiliative behavior in promoting social bonds. It also discusses the potential benefits of these skills in enhancing reproductive success.

Tipo: Apuntes

2013/2014

Subido el 15/05/2014

milumo-1
milumo-1 🇪🇸

3.8

(58)

4 documentos

1 / 8

Toggle sidebar

Documentos relacionados


Vista previa parcial del texto

¡Descarga Reflexive Empathy and Imitation: Social Bonds and Reproductive Success in Baboons - Prof. y más Apuntes en PDF de Etología solo en Docsity! Affiliation, empathy, and the origins of theory of mind Robert M. Seyfartha,1 and Dorothy L. Cheneyb Departments of aPsychology and bBiology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104 Edited by Francisco J. Ayala, University of California, Irvine, CA, and approved April 9, 2013 (received for review January 29, 2013) To understand the evolution of a Theory of Mind, we need to understand the selective factors that might have jumpstarted its initial evolution. We argue that a subconscious, reflexive appre- ciation of others’ intentions, emotions, and perspectives is at the roots of even the most complex forms of Theory of Mind and that these abilities may have evolved because natural selection has fa- vored individuals that are motivated to empathize with others and attend to their social interactions. These skills are adaptive because they are essential to forming strong, enduring social bonds,which in turn enhance reproductive success. We first review evidence from both humans and other animals indicating that reflexive and reflec- tive mental state attributions are inextricably linked and play a cru- cial role in promoting affiliative social bonds. We next describe results from free-ranging female baboons showing that individuals who show high rates of affiliative behavior form stronger social bondswith other females. These bonds, in turn, are linked tofitness. We then provide data from three different types of social challenges (male immigration, changes in grooming behavior after the death of a close relative, and responses during playback experiments), sug- gesting that females whomanifest high rates of affiliative behavior may also be more motivated to anticipate challenges, react adap- tively to setbacks, and respond appropriately to social interactions. personality | primates Do animals have a Theory of Mind (ToM)? Answers to thisquestion have tended to focus on two properties that might characterize a cognitive process. First, is an animal’s recognition of other individuals’ mental states reflexive and therefore, perhaps immediate and unconscious? Or is it reflective and therefore, more likely to be ruminative and conscious? Second, to what kinds of mental states are animals attentive:more rudimentary psychological states, like another individual’s gaze direction or its intentions, or more complex states, like another individual’s knowledge or beliefs? These distinctions are not easy to draw, even in humans, where reflective, conscious mindreading about others’ knowl- edge and beliefs is built on and develops gradually from re- flexive, unconscious recognition of, for example, another’s direction of gaze (1, 2). There is considerable evidence that many animals are re- flexively attuned to other individuals’ gaze, intentions, and emo- tions; however, the degree to which they are also reflectively aware of others’ knowledge and beliefs is less clear (3). Problems in assessment arise in part because whenever an animal behaves in ways that suggest an understanding of another’s knowledge, its behavior can often also be explained by simpler mechanisms, in- cluding learned contingences. A chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) who takes food that a rival cannot see might do so because she under- stands the relation between seeing and knowing or because she has learned the behavioral rule that a rival is motivated to defend food at which he is looking. Although experiments have attempted to distinguish between these explanations (4–7), results have not been easy to interpret. At the very least, they suggest that animals’ un- derstanding of others’ psychological states is quite different and perhaps less subject to conscious reflection than adult humans’. Whatever the explanation, it is clear that attempting to identify precise, definitive benchmarks of mental state attribution in ani- mals has proved to be more elusive and less productive than first hoped. Here, we take a slightly different approach to the question of mental state attribution in animals and consider the selective factors that might have favored the evolution of a rudimentary ToM. We begin by assuming that a full-blown ToM evolved from more rudimentary, reflexive forms that were themselves adaptive in their own right. As a first step in understanding the evolution of a ToM, therefore, we need to understand the selective factors that might have jumpstarted these rudimentary forms. We argue that a subconscious, reflexive appreciation of others’ intentions, emo- tions, and perspectives lies at the roots of even the most complex forms of ToM and that these abilities first evolved because natural selection favored individuals that were motivated to attend to other individuals’ social interactions and empathize with them. These skills were favored by selection because they are essential to forming strong, enduring social bonds, which in turn have been shown to enhance reproductive success. We therefore propose that the evolution of a ToM ultimately derives from its role in facilitating the formation of social bonds. We first review evidence from both humans and other animals indicating that reflexive and reflective mental state attributions are inextricably linked and play a crucial role in promoting affiliative social bonds. Then, using data on wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus), we suggest that individual variation in the motivation to attend to social interactions and react to social challenges is positively correlated with measures that have pre- viously been shown to be linked to the formation of social bonds and ultimately, enhanced reproductive success. Reflexive and Reflective Empathy in Animals and Humans Any attempt to determine whether an animal does or does not understand what another individual knows or thinks is inevitably confounded by the fact that the reflective processes associatedwith higher levels of ToM are closely linked to—and often hard to distinguish from—the more automatic, reflexive processes that underlie them (8, 9). Although we are consciously aware of the distinction between our own and others’mental states, we are often unaware of the many cues on which this awareness is based. For example, although higher cortical areas, like the prefrontal cortex, are activated when a human attempts to determine whether an- other individual can see something, initial responses to gaze di- rection and goal-directed behavior also activate more primitive areas of the brain, including the superior temporal sulcus (STS) and the amygdala. In both humans and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), the STS is particularly sensitive to the orientation of another individual’s eyes (10, 11). This paper results from the Arthur M. Sackler Colloquium of the National Academy of Sciences, “In the Light of Evolution VII: The Human Mental Machinery,” held January 10–12, 2013, at the Arnold and Mabel Beckman Center of the National Academies of Sciences and Engineering in Irvine, CA. The complete program and audio files of most presentations are available on the NAS Web site at www.nasonline.org/evolution_vii. Author contributions: R.M.S. and D.L.C. designed research, performed research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. The authors declare no conflict of interest. This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: seyfarth@psych.upenn.edu. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1301223110/-/DCSupplemental. www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1301223110 PNAS | June 18, 2013 | vol. 110 | suppl. 2 | 10349–10356 The same is true of intentional behavior. Although we have conscious access to our reflections about whether someone’s actions are accidental or intentional, many of the neuronal responses that contribute to our eventual decision are more subconscious. In both humans and monkeys, mirror neurons in the inferior parietal lobule are activated when both an individual performs a specific action and he observes someone else perform that action. Significantly, many neurons begin to fire before the other individual actually performs the action, suggesting that these neurons encode not only the specific motor act but also the actor’s intentions (12, 13). Thus, our ability to recognize that gaze has informative content, or to consider whether behavior is intentional, depends crucially on automatic, reflexive neuronal activity of which we are largely unaware. Similar results emerge in studies of empathy. Reflective, explicit empathy involves the ability to recognize emotional states like grief or fear in others without necessarily experiencing the same emo- tions oneself (8). However, reflective empathy evokes activity not just in the cortex but also more primitive areas of the brain shared with many animals, including the midbrain, the brainstem, and endocrine systems associated with reactivity, reward, and social attachment (14, 15). Althoughwe can distinguish between our own and others’ emotions, representations of emotions like pain, dis- gust, and shame in others also activate many of the same areas of the brain that are activated when we experience or imagine the same emotions ourselves (13). Feeling sympathy for or being nice to others is emotionally rewarding in part because it facilitates the release of dopamine, a neurotransmitter associated with personal reward (14). Trust, empathy, and sensitivity to others’ affective states are all facilitated by neuropeptides associated with attach- ment, maternal behavior, and pair bonding in animals, particu- larly oxytocin (16, 17). Thus, even the most reflective forms of empathy in humans are derived from and still strongly linked to more rudimentary forms. Similarly, reflective imitation involves the ability to recognize the goals and intentions of another and to understand that, to achieve the same goal, one must copy that individual’s actions. Human culture depends crucially on this ability, which is also shown to some degree by the great apes (18). Even humans, however, are largely unaware of many of the behaviors in others that they rou- tinely mimic. Like some animals, we have a reflexive, unconscious tendency to mimic the postures, mannerisms, and behavior of individuals with whom we are interacting. As already noted, in the motor domain the same mirror neurons are activated when an individual performs amovement as when he observes another engaged in that movement. Similarly, both human and nonhuman primates reflexively follow the gaze of others (19), and both human and macaque neonates copy others’ facial expressions (20, 21). The fact that such mimicking is associ- ated with empathy is exemplified by the phenomenon of contagious yawning. It is well known that viewing others yawn can elicit spon- taneous yawning in oneself. Even this apparently reflexive response, however, seems to vary according to an individual’s sensitivity to more reflective behavior, including face recognition and un- derstanding of others’mental states (22). Spontaneous yawning is rare or absent in children with autism spectrum disorder (23, 24). It also occurs at higher frequencies among kin and friends than among strangers, suggesting that contagious yawning is linked to and may also promote affiliation (25). These observations are not limited to humans: chimpanzees are also more likely to yawn in response to the yawns of familiar, as opposed to unfamiliar, individuals (26). A variety of other observations on what has been termed the chameleon effect (27) supports the view that reflexive mimicry is linked to the formation and maintenance of social bonds and has been favored by evolution because it promotes affiliation (28). Experiments suggest that people unconsciously mimic others when attempting to foster rapport and increase their frequency of mimicry when they are excluded from a group (29, 30). Being imitated increases helpful and affiliative behavior (31) and acti- vates areas in the brain associated with reward processing (32). In contrast, not being imitated increases cortisol levels (33). Similar observations have been obtained in nonhuman primates. Captive capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) are more willing to ap- proach and exchange tokens with a human who mimics their actions than one who does not (34). Male chimpanzees’ long-distance pant hoots become more similar acoustically as individuals spend more time together (35, 36), suggesting that call convergence is associated with, and may even promote, social affiliation. In practice, it is almost impossible to distinguish reflective empathy from more reflexive forms and learned negative asso- ciations (9). This problem is not surprising given neurological evidence that the two are closely linked. In an early experiment specifically designed to examine whether one monkey would re- spond to another’s distress, rhesus macaques were trained to pull chains to obtain a food reward. The apparatus was then rigged so that a monkey in an adjacent cage received a shock each time a particular chain was pulled. Most of the monkeys soon stopped pulling the chain that delivered the shock, even though doing so deprived them of a reward. They were especially likely to avoid the chain if they had previously received shocks themselves (37, 38). Although the monkeys’ responses might at first be inter- preted as evidence for reflective empathy, it seems as likely that they became distressed when they saw the other monkey being shocked because it was linked to a negative association for themselves. However, because even the most reflective forms of human empathy also evoke activity in more reflexive, primitive brain systems, these alternative explanations may be impossible to disambiguate. In a more recent experiment, macaques were given the option of delivering a reward to themselves, another monkey, or no one. Although subjects preferred to reward themselves over others, they nonetheless opted to reward their partner if the alternative was to reward no one. This preference was especially true if the partner was familiar (39). Significantly, the same brain areas that are activated in humans during such exchanges were also acti- vated in monkeys (40), and again—as in humans (41)—the monkeys’ vicarious reinforcement was enhanced if they first in- haled oxytocin (42). Finally, in another experiment rats were placed in an arena with a cagemate trapped in a translucent tube (43). The free rats quickly learned how to open the tube to liberate their cagemates, and they continued to do so even when given an alternative option to open a tube containing chocolate. (In the latter case, the rat opened both tubes and shared the chocolate.) It is possible that the free rats’ responses may have been provoked in part by their own elevated stress at hearing their cagemates’ alarm calls. However, given neurological evidence that witnessing distress in others activates many of the same brain areas as experiencing distress oneself, this distinction becomes difficult to disambiguate. In sum, a variety of evidence suggests that reflexive empathy and imitation in both humans and other animals have evolved because they promote affiliation and social bonding. Joint attention and joint action activate areas of the brain associated with the pro- cessing of reward, and they are facilitated by the release of oxytocin. Importantly, what seems to be rewarding to animals is not physical contact per se but the specific identity of the social partner. In so- cially monogamous tamarins (Saguinus oedipus), strongly bonded pairs exhibit higher oxytocin levels than more weakly bonded pairs (17). Among wild chimpanzees, urinary concentrations of oxytocin are higher after individuals groom with a closely bonded partner (both kin and nonkin) than with a less closely bonded partner (44). Evidently, grooming with a close friend or relative is more emo- tionally rewarding than engaging in the same behavior with a less preferred partner. 10350 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1301223110 Seyfarth and Cheney ensuing 3 mo, suggesting that they were unsuccessful in rebuilding their social network. This decrease occurred despite the fact that females who scored high on the Loner component tended to show a greater increase in GC levels than other females in the 2 wk after the death of a close relative, particularly when that relative was amother or adult daughter (rs= 0.771,N= 6,P> 0.10). In contrast, females who scored high on the Aloof and Nice components responded to the death of a close relative by grooming compara- tively more females than unaffected individuals. Variation in the strength of responses during playback experiments. Play- back experiments are designed to test subjects’ knowledge of other individuals’ dominance ranks and kinship as well as their memory of recent social interactions and their participants. Consider reconciliation, for example. Baboons often grunt to their opponents after aggression, and these grunts serve to restore opponents to baseline levels of tolerance (56). In an experiment designed to determine whether reconciliation by kin could serve as a proxy for direct reconciliation, victims were played the grunt of the close relative of a recent opponent. Subjects were signifi- cantly more likely to approach their opponent after hearing a grunt from their opponent’s relative (test condition) than after hearing a grunt from a female from a different matriline (control condition) (57). In so doing, subjects showed that they remembered not only the specific nature of a recent interaction and the identity of the participants but also the kinship relations (or close asso- ciates) of other females in their group. Thus, by responding more strongly during tests than control trials, subjects showed that they were not only reactive but also appropriately reactive, in the sense that they responded strongly only to relevant stimuli. For this analysis, we considered variation in females’ responses to playback stimuli in five previously conducted experiments that tested baboons’ memory of recent social interactions and knowledge of other individuals’ relationships (Table S1) (57–61). We used duration of looking toward the speaker in test compared with control trials as our dependent measure, because this re- sponse was used in all experiments. Because the strength of subjects’ responses varied across experiments, we ranked each subject’s duration of response in each experiment relative to re- sponse duration of other subjects. Thus, a subject who responded more strongly in the test vs. the control condition received a high positive ranking, whereas a subject that respondedmore strongly in the control condition received a negative ranking. The correlations between strength of response and Aloof, Loner, and Nice scores were all positive, but only the Nice scores reached statistical significance (Aloof: β = 0.381, SE = 0.580, t = 0.657, P > 0.10; Loner: β = 0.625, SE = 0.634, t = 0.986, P > 0.10; Nice: β = 1.250, SE = 0.566, t = 2.246, P = 0.027) (Fig. 3). Thus, although most females responded more strongly during test than control trials, females who scored high on the Nice component were the most responsive. Discussion: Social Challenges. Previous analyses (50) showed that females scoring high on the Nice component have stronger social bonds with other females. The data presented here suggest that, by three independent measures, these individuals may also be more responsive to social challenges and more motivated to attend to social interactions within their group (Table 1). First, females who score high on Nice may better anticipate threats that have yet to occur. Such females showed a greater anticipatory increase in GC levels at the arrival of a potentially infanticidal immigrant, suggesting that they recognized the threat that such a male represented even before he attacked any infants. Second, females who score high on Nice may respond more adaptively to setbacks that have occurred. Females with high Nice and Aloof scores had more grooming partners than unaffected R el at iv e ch an ge in n um be r Aloof Loner R el at iv e ch an ge in n um be r Nice R el at iv e ch an ge in n um be r Fig. 2. The relative number of a female’s different grooming partners in the 3 mo after the death of a close relative (mother, adult daughter, or sister) compared with the mean number of grooming partners for all other females in those months (controlled for reproductive state). n = 18 females who lost from one to three close relatives for a total sample of 24 female-y. One outlier was removed. Legend is the same as in Fig. 1. Seyfarth and Cheney PNAS | June 18, 2013 | vol. 110 | suppl. 2 | 10353 females after the death of a close relative, suggesting that they were attempting to identify new social partners. In contrast, females who scored high on Loner had fewer partners than other females, suggesting that they made no such effort. Finally, there was some indication that females who scored high on Nice were more appropriately reactive during playback experiments than other females, perhaps because they were generally more atten- tive to social interactions and events in their group. It is important to emphasize that all of the observed differences were small. As a group, females responded positively to each of the three challenges, so it is not surprising that individual varia- tion in response strength was subject to a ceiling effect. The attributes associated with females who scored high on the Nice component were not unique to these individuals; rather, such females seemed more consistently to show strong anticipatory and reactive responses to challenges. Clearly, however, this hy- pothesis will need to be tested in future research. Several recent studies of birds have investigated the relation- ship between problem solving ability and fitness. Results suggest that variation in problem solving does not result solely from differences in motivation or reactivity but instead may reflect genuine differences in cognitive ability (62–64). In contrast, we have no evidence that female baboons vary in their cognitive abilities—that, for example, Nice females are, by some measure, more skilled at problem solving. Instead, data suggest that dif- ferences in personality styles may be associated with greater re- sponsiveness to social challenges and greater motivation to attend to, recall, and anticipate social interactions. In a somewhat similar study of captive rhesus macaques, males who scored high on a sociability index (defined as the motivation to seek out others) experienced better health. These males also coped better both physiologically and behaviorally during unstable social conditions, and they seemed more motivated than other males to manage unpredictable social circumstances (65). In contrast to males that scored low on the sociability index, they seemed to find social interactions rewarding rather than aversive. Similar differences were observed among Nice and Loner female baboons. Conclusions Human behavior during cooperative interactions is often con- trasted with behavior of chimpanzees, which in captivity show little evidence of prosocial behavior. Indeed, experiments explicitly R an k in tr ia ls R an k in tr ia ls R an k in tr ia ls Aloof Loner Nice Fig. 3. Variation in females’ responses to playback stimuli in five different experiments (Table S1). Subjects were ranked according to the strength of their response in experimental trials minus control trials. n = 33 females in one to five experiments for a total of 73 subjects. One outlier was removed. Legend is the same as in Fig. 1. Table 1. Signs of the β-coefficients (regression slopes) of the relation between personality component scores (Nice, Aloof, and Loner) and three social challenges (details in the text) Nice Aloof Loner Male immigration + − − Change in grooming partner number after relative’s death + + − Playback experiments +* + + *P < 0.05. 10354 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1301223110 Seyfarth and Cheney designed to compare the behavior of children and chimpanzees suggest that humans are unique not only in their motivation to participate in activities that involve shared goals and joint action but also their concern for the welfare of others (66–68). Most forms of apparent prosociality and empathy in animals can be explained functionally in terms of parental behavior, nepotism, or direct benefit. There is little evidence in animals—even apes—for the kind of reflective empathy that permits an individual to disasso- ciate his own emotions and needs from others’ or be sensitive to another’s welfare independent of his own (69). However, although cooperation among humans clearly differs from cooperation among animals in many nontrivial ways, more naturalistic studies suggest that the contrasts are not as stark as initially proposed. For example, wild chimpanzees engage in a variety of cooperative, sometimes risky ventures with long-term social partners, including grooming, food sharing, and border patrols (70). These interactions seem to be emotionally rewarding. As already mentioned, both male and female chimpanzees experi- ence elevations in urinary oxytocin after grooming with a preferred partner (44), and males that share meat with others experience a significant drop in testosterone (71). Thus, chimpanzees in the wild regularly engage in joint, cooperative action and seem to derive pleasure and reduced tension from what are, arguably, rather prosocial activities. We have suggested that a full-blown ToM evolved from a rudi- mentary form that is reflexively attentive and sensitive to others’ attention, emotions, and intentions. This rudimentary form of shared attention is widespread in many animals, still present in humans, and difficult to disambiguate—behaviorally and neuro- logically—from humans’ more derived, reflective ToM. We can explain the evolution of rudimentary ToM by noting that it facili- tates attentiveness to others’ emotional states and thereby pro- motes the formation of strong social bonds, which are linked to fitness. Shared attention and sensitivity to others’ emotions may promote bond formation, because they include a subcon- scious tendency to mimic individuals with whom we are interacting. When two individuals interact, imitation makes the behavior of each partner more predictable to the other, and predictability and control are major modulators of stress (72). We may therefore find individuals who imitate us attractive in part because they are predictable. In sum, a rudimentary ToMmay have evolved because, in social animals, reflexive empathy and joint attention are both emotion- ally rewarding and adaptive. Individuals who are motivated to at- tend to others’ behavioral, attentional, and emotional states are more likely to interact positively with others and form stronger social bonds. Strong social bonds, in turn, contribute to reproductive success. Individual variation in the strength of social bonds, stress levels, and responsiveness to social challenges may stem less from variation in cognitive ability than variation in the motivation to at- tend to and participate in social interactions. Methods Data were derived from a long-term study of wild chacma baboons (P. hamadryas ursinus) in the Moremi Game Reserve, Botswana. The group had been observed since 1978. Maternal kinship was known for all indi- viduals. The primary causes of mortality were infanticide and predation (details in ref. 1). Statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical software (version 2.15; R Foundation for Statistical Computing). For exploratory principle component analysis, we used the principle function in the psych packagewith the default varimax rotation. To test for a relation between scores on principle components and dependent variables, we used linear mixed models (lmer in R), entering female identification and year as random factors. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank the Office of the President, Republic of Botswana and theDepartment ofWildlife andNational Parks for permission to work in the Moremi Reserve. We thank J. Beehner, T. Bergman, C. Crockford, A. Engh, M. Heesen, L. Moscovice, R. Wittig, K. Seyfarth, C. Shaw, M. Mokopi, and A. Mokopi for assistance with data collection and J. Silk and two anonymous reviewers for comments. Research was supported by the National Science Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, the Leakey Foundation, the National Geographic Society, and the University of Pennsylvania. Research was approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Pennsylvania (Protocol No. 19001). 1. Onishi KH, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308(5719):255–258. 2. Apperly IA (2012) What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and in- dividual differences. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 65(5):825–839. 3. Cheney D, Seyfarth R (2007) Baboon Metaphysics: The Evolution of a Social Mind (Univ of Chicago Press, Chicago). 4. Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition 109(2):224–234. 5. Bugnyar T (2011) Knower-guesser differentiation in ravens: Others’ viewpoints mat- ter. Proc Biol Sci 278(1705):634–640. 6. MacLean E, Hare B (2012) Bonobos and chimpanzees infer the target of another’s attention. Anim Behav 83(2):345–353. 7. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Mundry R, Zuberbühler K (2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Curr Biol 22(2):142–146. 8. Hecht EE, Patterson R, Barbey AK (2012) What can other animals tell us about human social cognition? An evolutionary perspective on reflective and reflexive processing. Front Hum Neurosci 6:224. 9. de Waal FB (2012) The antiquity of empathy. Science 336(6083):874–876. 10. Jellema T, Baker CI, Wicker B, Perrett DI (2000) Neural representation for the per- ception of the intentionality of actions. Brain Cogn 44(2):280–302. 11. Klein JT, Shepherd SV, Platt ML (2009) Social attention and the brain. Curr Biol 19(20): R958–R962. 12. Fogassi L, et al. (2005) Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention un- derstanding. Science 308(5722):662–667. 13. Rizzolatti G, Maddalena F-D (2009) Handbook of Neuroscience for the Behavioral Sciences, eds Rizzolatti G, Maddalena F-D (Wiley, New York), Vol 1, pp 337–357. 14. Decety J (2011) The neuroevolution of empathy. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1231:35–45. 15. Decety J, Jackson PL (2004) The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 3(2):71–100. 16. Carter CS, Grippo AJ, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Ruscio MG, Porges SW (2008) Oxytocin, vasopressin and sociality. Prog Brain Res 170:331–336. 17. Snowdon CT, et al. (2010) Variation in oxytocin is related to variation in affiliative behavior in monogamous, pairbonded tamarins. Horm Behav 58(4):614–618. 18. Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2007) Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev Sci 10(4):F31–F38. 19. Shepherd SV, Klein JT, Deaner RO, Platt ML (2009) Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106(23):9489–9494. 20. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198(4312):75–78. 21. Ferrari PF, et al. (2006) Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biol 4(9):e302. 22. Platek SM, Critton SR, Myers TE, Gallup GG (2003) Contagious yawning: The role of self-awareness and mental state attribution. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 17(2):223–227. 23. Helt MS, Eigsti IM, Snyder PJ, Fein DA (2010) Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development. Child Dev 81(5):1620–1631. 24. Senju A, et al. (2007) Absence of contagious yawning in children with autism spec- trum disorder. Biol Lett 3(6):706–708. 25. Norscia I, Palagi E (2011) Yawn contagion and empathy in Homo sapiens. PLoS One 6(12):e28472. 26. Campbell MW, de Waal FB (2011) Ingroup-outgroup bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. PLoS One 6(4):e18283. 27. Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910. 28. Lakin J, Jefferis V, Cheng C, Chartrand T (2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J Nonverbal Behav 27(2):145–162. 29. Lakin JL, Chartrand TL (2003) Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affil- iation and rapport. Psychol Sci 14(4):334–339. 30. Lakin JL, Chartrand TL, Arkin RM (2008) I am too just like you: Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusion. Psychol Sci 19(8):816–822. 31. Van Baaren R, Holland R, Kawakami K, van Knippenberg A (2004) Mimicry and pro- social behavior. Psychol Sci 15(1):71–74. 32. Kühn S, et al. (2010) Why do I like you when you behave like me? Neural mechanisms mediating positive consequences of observing someone being imitated. Soc Neurosci 5(4):384–392. 33. Kouzakova M, van Baaren R, van Knippenberg A (2010) Lack of behavioral imitation in human interactions enhances salivary cortisol levels. Horm Behav 57(4–5):421–426. 34. Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Visalberghi E, Ferrari PF (2009) Capuchin monkeys display af- filiation toward humans who imitate them. Science 325(5942):880–883. 35. Mitani JC, Hunley KL, Murdoch ME (1999) Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: A reassessment. Am J Primatol 47(2):133–151. 36. Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigilant L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243. 37. Masserman JH, Wechkin S, Terris W (1964) “Altruistic” behavior in rhesus monkeys. Am J Psychiatry 121:584–585. Seyfarth and Cheney PNAS | June 18, 2013 | vol. 110 | suppl. 2 | 10355
Docsity logo



Copyright © 2024 Ladybird Srl - Via Leonardo da Vinci 16, 10126, Torino, Italy - VAT 10816460017 - All rights reserved